中国有sleepsleep number中国吗

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
This article is about the section in the brain. For the fish genus Hippocampus, see . For other uses, see .
The hippocampus (named after its resemblance to the , from the
?ππ?καμπο?, "seahorse" from ?ππο? hippos, "horse" and κ?μπο? kampos, "sea monster") is a major component of the
and other . Humans and other mammals have two hippocampi, one in each . The hippocampus belongs to the
and plays important roles in the consolidation of information from
to , and in
that enables navigation. The hippocampus is located under the
() and in primates in the . It contains two main interlocking parts: the
(also called Ammon's horn) and the .
(and other forms of ), the hippocampus is one of the first regions of the br
are included among the early symptoms. Damage to the hippocampus can also result from
(), , or . People with extensive, bilateral hippocampal damage may experience
(the inability to form and retain new ).
as , the hippocampus has been studied extensively as part of a brain system responsible for
and navigation. Many
hippocampus respond as : that is, they fire bursts of
when the animal passes through a specific part of its environment. Hippocampal place cells interact extensively with , whose activity acts as an inertial compass, and conjecturally with
in the neighboring .
Since different
are neatly organized into layers in the hippocampus, it has frequently been used as a
for studying . The form of
(LTP) was first discovered to occur in the hippocampus and has often been studied in this structure. LTP is widely believed to be one of the main neural mechanisms by which memories are stored in the brain.
Image 1: The human hippocampus and
compared with a
The earliest description of the ridge running along the floor of the
comes from the Venetian anatomist
(1587), who likened it first to a
and then to a
(: hippocampus from : ?ππο?, "horse" and κ?μπο?, "sea monster"). The German anatomist Duvernoy (1729), the first to illustrate the structure, also wavered between "seahorse" and "silkworm." "Ram's horn" was proposed by the Danish anatomist
in 1732; and a decade later his fellow Parisian, the surgeon de Garengeot, used "cornu Ammonis" – horn of (the ancient Egyptian god) , who was often represented as having a ram's head. This has survived in abbreviated form as CA in naming the subfields of the hippocampus. Another reference appeared with the term , which may date back to
in 1672, introducing a comparison with the shape of the folded back forelimbs and webbed feet of the , a sea monster with a horse's forequarters and a fish's tail. The hippocampus was then described as pes hippocampi major, with an adjacent bulge in the , described as the pes hippocampi minor and later renamed as the . The renaming of the hippocampus as hippocampus major, and the calcar avis as hippocampus minor, has been attributed to
systematising nomenclature of parts of the brain in 1786.
mistakenly used the term
in 1779, and was followed by some other authors until
resolved this error in 1829. In 1861 the hippocampus minor became the centre of a dispute over
and , satirised as the . The term hippocampus minor fell from use in anatomy textbooks, and was officially removed in the
of 1895. Today, the structure is just called the hippocampus, with the term Cornu Ammonis surviving in the names of the hippocampal subfields CA1–CA4.
The term limbic system was introduced in 1952 by
to describe the set of structures that line the edge of the cortex (Latin limbus meaning border): These include the hippocampus, , , and . Paul MacLean later suggested that the limbic structures comprise the neural basis of emotion. The hippocampus is anatomically connected to parts of the brain that are involved with emotional behavior—the , the
, and the , and is generally accepted to be part of the limbic system.
Main article:
Image 2: Cross-section of cerebral hemisphere showing structure and location of hippocampus
Image 3: Coronal section of the brain of a macaque monkey, showing hippocampus (circled)
The hippocampus can be seen as a ridge of , elevating from the floor of each
in the region of the inferior or temporal horn. This ridge can also be seen as an inward fold of the
into the . The hippocampus can only be seen in
as it is concealed by the . The cortex thins from six layers to the three or four layers that make up the hippocampus.
is used to refer to the
and its related parts. However, there is no consensus as to what parts are included. Sometimes the hippocampus is said to include the dentate gyrus and the subiculum. Some references include the
in the hippocampal formation, and others also include the presubiculum, , and . The neural layout and pathways within the hippocampal formation are very similar in all mammals.
The hippocampus, including the , has the shape of a curved tube, which has been compared to a seahorse, and a ram's horn (Cornu Ammonis). Its abbreviation CA is used in naming the hippocampal subfields: , , , and . It can be distinguished as an area where the cortex narrows into a single layer of densely packed , which curl into a tight U shape. One edge of the "U," – CA4, is embedded into the backward-facing, flexed dentate gyrus. The hippocampus is described as having an
part (in ) or a
part in other animals. Both parts are of similar composition but belong to different neural circuits. In the rat, the two hippocampi resemble a pair of bananas, joined at the stems by the
(also called the hippocampal commissure). In , the part of the hippocampus at the bottom, near the base of the , is much broader than the part at the top. This means that in cross-section the hippocampus can show a number of different shapes, depending on the angle and location of the cut.
In a cross-section of the hippocampus, including the dentate gyrus, several layers will be shown. The dentate gyrus has three layers of cells (or four if the hilus is included). The layers are from the outer in - the molecular layer, the inner molecular layer, the granular layer, and the hilus. The CA3 in the hippocampus proper has the following cell layers known as strata: lacunosum-moleculare, radiatum, lucidum, pyramidal, and oriens. CA2 and CA1 also have four each of these layers and not the .
The input to the hippocampus (from varying cortical and subcortical structures) comes from the
via the . The entorhinal cortex (EC) is strongly and reciprocally connected with many cortical and subcortical structures as well as with the brainstem. Different , (from the anterior and midline groups), the , the supramamillary nucleus of the hypothalamus, and the
all send axons to the EC, so that it serves as the interface between the neocortex and the other connections, and the hippocampus.
The EC, is located in the , a cortical region adjacent to the hippocampus. This gyrus conceals the hippocampus. The parahippocampal gyrus also includes the
which plays an important role in the visual recognition of complex objects. There is also substantial evidence that it makes a contribution to memory, which can be distinguished from the contribution of the hippocampus. It is apparent that complete
occurs only when both the hippocampus and the parahippocampus are damaged.
Image 4: Basic circuit of the hippocampus, as drawn by
DG: dentate gyrus. Sub: subiculum. EC: entorhinal cortex.
The perforant path is the main output pathway of the entorhinal cortex (EC). The flow of information in the hippocampus is largely unidirectional. Axons from the pyramidal cells (in EC layer II) perforate the subiculum to project mainly to the granular layer in the dentate gyrus (first synaptic connection). Some axons project to CA3 and a lesser number project to CA1. The dentate granule cell axons, called , pass on the information, to the dendrites of CA3 pyramidal cells (second synaptic connection). From there, CA3 axons called
leave the deep part of the
and loop up to the apical dendrites and then extend to CA1. Axons from CA1 then project back to the entorhinal cortex (third synaptic connection) completing the .
in CA3 receive
input from the pyramidal cells and then give an
feedback to the pyramidal cells. This recurrent inhibition is a simple feedback circuit that can dampen excitatory responses in the hippocampus. The pyramidal cells gives a recurrent excitation which is an important mechanism found in some memory processing microcircuits.
Several other connections play important roles in hippocampal function. Beyond the output to the EC, additional output pathways go to other cortical areas including the . A major output goes via the
and to the
of the hypothalamus (which the fornix interconnects with the hippocampus). The hippocampus receives modulatory input from the , , and
systems, and from the
to field CA1. A very important projection comes from the medial septal nucleus, which sends , and
stimulating fibers (GABAergic fibers) to all parts of the hippocampus. The inputs from the medial septal nucleus play a key role in controlling the physiological stat destruction of this nucleus abolishes the hippocampal
and severely impairs certain types of memory.
Image 5: Hippocampal location and regions
Areas of the hippocampus are shown to be functionally and anatomically distinct. The dorsal hippocampus (DH), ventral hippocampus (VH) and intermediate hippocampus serve different functions, project with differing pathways, and have varying degrees of place cells. The dorsal hippocampus serves for spatial memory, verbal memory, and learning of conceptual information. Using the , lesions in the DH were shown to cause spatial memory impairment while VH lesions did not. Its projecting pathways include the medial septal nucleus and supramammillary nucleus. The dorsal hippocampus also has more place cells than both the ventral and intermediate hippocampal regions.
The intermediate hippocampus has overlapping characteristics with both the ventral and dorsal hippocampus. Using
methods, Cenquizca and Swanson (2007) located the moderate projections to two primary olfactory cortical areas and prelimbic areas of the . This region has the smallest number of place cells. The ventral hippocampus functions in fear conditioning and affective processes. Anagnostaras et al. (2002) showed that alterations to the ventral hippocampus reduced the amount of information sent to the amygdala by the dorsal and ventral hippocampus, consequently altering fear conditioning in rats. Historically, the earliest widely held hypothesis was that the hippocampus is involved in . This idea was cast into doubt by a series of anatomical studies that did not find any direct projections to the hippocampus from the . However, later work did confirm that the olfactory bulb does project into the ventral part of the lateral entorhinal cortex, and field CA1 in the ventral hippocampus sends axons to the main olfactory bulb, the anterior olfactory nucleus, and to the primary olfactory cortex. There continues to be some interest in hippocampal olfactory responses, in particular, the role of the hippocampus in memory for odors, but few specialists today believe that olfaction is its primary function.
Over the years, three main ideas of hippocampal function have dominated the literature: , , and spatial cognition. The behavioral inhibition theory (caricatured by
as "slam on the brakes!") was very popular up to the 1960s. It derived much of its justification from two observations: first, that animals with hippocampa second, that animals with hippocampal damage often have difficulty learning to inhibit responses that they have previously been taught, especially if the response requires remaining quiet as in a passive avoidance test.
developed this line of thought into a full-fledged theory of the role of the hippocampus in anxiety. The inhibition theory is currently the least popular of the three.
The second major line of thought relates the hippocampus to memory. Although it had historical precursors, this idea derived its main impetus from a famous report by
describing the results of surgical destruction of the hippocampi (in an attempt to relieve ), in , known until his death in 2008 as "Patient H.M." The unexpected outcome of the surgery was
Molaison was unable to form new
after his surgery and could not remember any events that occurred just before his surgery, but he did retain memories of events that occurred many years earlier extending back into his childhood. This case attracted such widespread professional interest that Molaison became the most intensively studied subject in medical history. In the ensuing years, other patients with similar levels of hippocampal damage and amnesia (caused by accident or disease) have also been studied, and thousands of experiments have studied the physiology of activity-driven changes in
in the hippocampus. There is now universal agreement that the hippocampi play some sort of impo however, the precise nature of this role remains widely debated.
The third important theory of hippocampal function relates the hippocampus to space. The spatial theory was originally championed by O'Keefe and Nadel, who were influenced by
theories about "" in humans and animals. O'Keefe and his student Dostrovsky in 1971 discovered neurons in the rat hippocampus that appeared to them to show activity related to the rat's location within its environment. Despite
from other investigators, O'Keefe and his co-workers, especially , continued to investigate this question, in a line of work that eventually led to their very influential 1978 book The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. There is now almost universal agreement that hippocampal function plays an important role in spatial coding, but the details are widely debated.
Later research has focused on trying to bridge the disconnect between the two main views of hippocampal function as being split between memory and spatial cognition. In some studies, these areas have been expanded to the point of near convergence. In an attempt to reconcile the two disparate views it is suggested that a broader view of the hippocampal function is taken and seen to have a role that encompasses both the organisation of experience (), (as per Tolman's original concept in 1948) and the directional behaviour seen as being involved in all areas of cognition. So that the function of the hippocampus can be viewed as a broader system that incorporates both the memory and the spatial perspectives in its role that involves the use of a wide scope of cognitive maps. This relates to the
born of Tolman's original goal of identifying the complex cognitive mechanisms and purposes that guided behaviour.
It has also been proposed that the spiking activity of hippocampal neurons is associated spatially, and it was suggested that the mechanisms of memory and planning both evolved from mechanisms of navigation and that their neuronal algorithms were basically the same.
Many studies have made use of
techniques such as
(fMRI) and a functional role in approach-avoidance conflict has been noted. The anterior hippocampus is seen to be involved in decision-making under approach-avoidance conflict processing. It is suggested that the memory, spatial cognition, and conflict processing functions may be seen as working together and not mutually exclusive.
generally agree that the hippocampus plays an important role in the formation of new
about experienced events ( or ). Part of this function is hippocampal involvement in the detection of new events, places and stimuli. Some researchers regard the hippocampus as part of a larger
memory system responsible for general
(memories that can be explicitly verbalized—these would include, for example,
in addition to episodic memory).
the brain has a hippocampus in each . If damage to the hippocampus occurs in only one hemisphere, leaving the structure intact in the other hemisphere, the brain can retain near-normal memory functioning. Severe damage to the hippocampi in both hemispheres results in profound difficulties in forming new memories () and often also affects memories formed before the damage occurred (). Although the retrograde effect normally extends many years back before the brain damage, in some cases older memories remain. This retention of older memories leads to the idea that consolidation over time involves the transfer of memories out of the hippocampus to other parts of the brain. Experiments using intrahippocampal transplantation of hippocampal cells in primates with neurotoxic lesions of the hippocampus have shown that the hippocampus is required for the formation and recall, but not the storage, of memories.
Damage to the hippocampus does not affect some types of memory, such as the ability to learn new skills (playing a musical instrument or solving certain types of puzzles, for example). This fact suggests that such abilities depend on different types of memory () and different brain regions. Furthermore, amnesic patients frequently show "implicit" memory for experiences even in the absence of conscious knowledge. For example, patients asked to guess which of two faces they have seen most recently may give the correct answer most of the time in spite of stating that they have never seen either of the faces before. Some researchers distinguish between conscious recollection, which depends on the hippocampus, and familiarity, which depends on portions of the medial temporal lobe.
Main article:
Image 6: Spatial firing patterns of 8 place cells recorded from the CA1 layer of a rat. The rat ran back and forth along an elevated track, stopping at each end to eat a small food reward. Dots indicate positions where action potentials were recorded, with color indicating which neuron emitted that action potential.
Studies on freely moving rats and mice have shown many hippocampal
that cluster in place fields, and these fire bursts of
when the animal passes through a particular location. This place-related neural activity in the hippocampus has also been reported in monkeys that were moved around a room whilst in a restraint chair. However, the place cells may have fired in relation to where the monkey was looking rather that to its actual location in the room. Over many years, many studies have been carried out on place-responses in rodents, that have given a large amount of information. Place cell responses are shown by
in the hippocampus and by
in the . Other cells in smaller proportion are inhibitory
and these often show place-related variations in their firing rate which are much weaker. There is little if any spatial topography i in general, cells lying next to each other in the hippocampus have uncorrelated spatial firing patterns. Place cells are typically almost silent when a rat is moving around outside the place field but reach sustained rates as high as 40 , when the rat is near the center. Neural activity sampled from 30 to 40 randomly chosen place cells carries enough information to allow a rat's location to be reconstructed with high confidence. The size of place fields varies in a gradient along the length of the hippocampus, with cells at the dorsal end showing the smallest fields, cells near the center showing larger fields, and cells at the ventral tip showing fields that cover the entire environment. In some cases, the firing rate of hippocampal cells depends not only on place but also on the direction a rat is moving, the destination toward which it is traveling, or other task-related variables.
In humans, cells with location-specific firing patterns have been reported during a study of patients with . They were undergoing an invasive procedure to localize the source of their , with a view to surgical resection. The patients had diagnostic electrodes implanted in their hippocampus and then used a computer to move around in a
town. Similar
studies in navigation have shown the hippocampus to be active. A study was carried out on taxi drivers. London’s
drivers need to learn the locations of a large number of places and the fastest routes between them in order to pass a strict test known as
in order to gain a license to operate. A study showed that the posterior part of the hippocampus is larger in these drivers than in the general public, and that a positive correlation exists between the length of time served as a driver and the increase in the volume of this part. It was also found the total volume of the hippocampus was unchanged, as the increase seen in the posterior part was made at the expense of the anterior part, which showed a relative decrease in size. There have been no reported adverse effects from this disparity in hippocampal proportions.
There are several navigational cells in the brain that are either in the hippocampus itself or are strongly connected to it, such as the
present in the . Together these cells form a network that serves as spatial memory. The first of such cells discovered in the 1970s were the place cells, which led to the idea of the hippocampus acting to give a neural representation of the environment in a . When the hippocampus is dysfunctional, ori people may have difficulty in remembering how they arrived at a location and how to proceed further. Getting lost is a common symptom of amnesia. Studies with animals have shown that an intact hippocampus is required for initial learning and long-term retention of some
tasks, in particular ones that require finding the way to a hidden goal. Other cells have been discovered since the finding of the place cells in the rodent brain that are either in the hippocampus or the entorhinal cortex. These have been assigned as ,
and . Speed cells are thought to provide input to the hippocampal grid cells.
Approach-avoidance conflict happens when a situation is presented that can either be rewarding or punishing, and the ensuing decision-making has been associated with . fMRI findings from studies in approach-avoidance decision-making found evidence for a functional role that is not explained by either long-term memory or spatial cognition. Overall findings showed that the anterior hippocampus is sensitive to conflict, and that it may be part of a larger cortical and subcortical network seen to be important in decision making in uncertain conditions.
A review makes reference to a number of studies that show the involvement of the hippocampus in conflict tasks. The authors suggest that a challenge is to understand how conflict processing relates to the functions of spatial navigation and memory and how all of these functions need not be mutually exclusive.
Image 7: Examples of rat hippocampal EEG and CA1 neural activity in the theta (awake/behaving) and LIA (slow-wave sleep) modes. Each plot shows 20 seconds of data, with a hippocampal EEG trace at the top, spike rasters from 40 simultaneously recorded CA1 pyramidal cells in the middle (each raster line represents a different cell), and a plot of running speed at the bottom. The top plot represents a time period during which the rat was actively searching for scattered food pellets. For the bottom plot the rat was asleep.
The hippocampus shows two major "modes" of activity, each associated with a distinct pattern of
activity and waves of electrical activity as measured by an
(EEG). These modes are named after the EEG patterns associated with them:
(LIA). The main characteristics described below are for the rat, which is the animal most extensively studied.
The theta mode appears during states of active, alert behavior (especially locomotion), and also during
(dreaming) sleep. In the theta mode, the EEG is dominated by large regular waves with a
of 6 to 9 , and the main groups of hippocampal neurons ( and ) show sparse population activity, which means that in any short time interval, the great majority of cells are silent, while the small remaining fraction fire at relatively high rates, up to 50 spikes in one second for the most active of them. An active cell typically stays active for half a second to a few seconds. As the rat behaves, the active cells fall silent and new cells become active, but the overall percentage of active cells remains more or less constant. In many situations, cell activity is determined largely by the spatial location of the animal, but other behavioral variables also clearly influence it.
The LIA mode appears during
(non-dreaming) sleep, and also during states of waking immobility such as resting or eating. In the LIA mode, the EEG is dominated by sharp waves that are randomly timed large deflections of the EEG signal lasting for 25–50 milliseconds. Sharp waves are frequently generated in sets, with sets containing up to 5 or more individual sharp waves and lasting up to 500 ms. The spiking activity of neurons within the hippocampus is highly correlated with sharp wave activity. Most neurons decrease their firing rate however, during a sharp wave, there is a dramatic increase in firing rate in up to 10% of the hippocampal population
These two hippocampal activity modes can be seen in primates as well as rats, with the exception that it has been difficult to see robust theta rhythmicity in the primate hippocampus. There are, however, qualitatively similar sharp waves and similar state-dependent changes in neural population activity.
Main article:
Image 8: Example of a one-second EEG theta wave
Because of its densely packed neural layers, of all the brain structures which generate the hippocampal theta rhythm, the hippocampus generates some of the largest EEG signals as
. In some situations the EEG is dominated by regular waves at 3 to 10 Hz, often continuing for many seconds. These reflect subthreshold
and strongly modulate the spiking of hippocampal neurons and synchronise across the hippocampus in a travelling wave pattern. The
is a relay of
in the hippocampus that interacts with many brain regions. From
it has been proposed that the trisynaptic circuit generates the hippocampal theta rhythm.
Theta rhythmicity is very obvious in rabbits and rodents and also clearly present in cats and dogs. Whether theta can be seen in primates is not yet clear. In
(the animals that have been the most extensively studied), theta is seen mainly in two conditions: first, when an animal is walking or in some other way actively interacting w second, during . The function of theta has not yet been convincingly explained although numerous theories have been proposed. The most popular hypothesis has been to relate it to learning and memory. An example would be the phase with which theta rhythms, at the time of stimulation of a neuron, shape the effect of that stimulation upon its synapses. What is meant here is that theta rhythms may affect those aspects of learning and memory that are dependent upon . It is well established that lesions of the —the central node of the theta system—cause severe disruptions of memory. However, the medial septum is more than just the it is also the main source of
projections to the hippocampus. It has not been established that septal lesions exert their effects specifically by eliminating the theta rhythm.
Main article:
During sleep or during resting, when an animal is not engaged with its surroundings, the hippocampal EEG shows a pattern of irregular slow waves, somewhat larger in amplitude than theta waves. This pattern is occasionally interrupted by large surges called sharp waves. These events are associated with bursts of spike activity lasting 50 to 100 milliseconds in pyramidal cells of CA3 and CA1. They are also associated with short-lived high-frequency EEG oscillations called "ripples", with frequencies in the range 150 to 200 Hz in rats, and together they are known as . Sharp waves are most frequent during sleep when they occur at an average rate of around 1 per second (in rats) but in a very irregular temporal pattern. Sharp waves are less frequent during inactive waking states and are usually smaller. Sharp waves have also been observed in humans and monkeys. In macaques, sharp waves are robust but do not occur as frequently as in rats.
One of the most interesting aspects of sharp waves is that they appear to be associated with memory. Wilson and McNaughton 1994, and numerous later studies, reported that when hippocampal place cells have overlapping spatial firing fields (and therefore often fire in near-simultaneity), they tend to show correlated activity during sleep following the behavioral session. This enhancement of correlation, commonly known as reactivation, has been found to occur mainly during sharp waves. It has been proposed that sharp waves are, in fact, reactivations of neural activity patterns that were memorized during behavior, driven by strengthening of synaptic connections within the hippocampus. This idea forms a key component of the "two-stage memory" theory, advocated by Buzsáki and others, which proposes that memories are stored within the hippocampus during behavior and then later transferred to the
during sleep. Sharp waves in
are seen as persistently repeated stimulations by presynaptic cells, of postsynaptic cells that are suggested to drive synaptic changes in the cortical targets of hippocampal output pathways. Suppression of sharp waves and ripples in sleep or during immobility can interfere with memories expressed at the level of the behavior, nonetheless, the newly formed CA1 place cell code can re-emerge even after a sleep with abolished sharp waves and ripples, in spatially non-demanding tasks.
Since at least the time of
(), psychologists have speculated that the brain stores memory by altering the strength of connections between neurons that are simultaneously active. This idea was formalized by
in 1949, but for many years remained unexplained. In 1973,
described a phenomenon in the rabbit hippocampus that appeared to meet Hebb's specifications: a change in synaptic responsiveness induced by brief strong activation and lasting for hours or days or longer. This phenomenon was soon referred to as
(LTP). As a candidate mechanism for , LTP has since been studied intensively, and a great deal has been learned about it. However, the complexity and variety of the intracellular signalling cascades that can trigger LTP is acknowledged as preventing a more complete understanding.
The hippocampus is a particularly favorable site for studying LTP because of its densely packed and sharply defined layers of neurons, but similar types of activity-dependent synaptic change have also been observed in many other brain areas. The best-studied form of LTP has been seen in CA1 of the hippocampus and occurs at synapses that terminate on
and use the
. The synaptic changes depend on a special type of , the , a
which has the special property of allowing calcium to enter the postsynaptic spine only when presynaptic activation and postsynaptic
occur at the same time. Drugs that interfere with NMDA receptors block LTP and have major effects on some types of memory, especially spatial memory.
to disable the LTP mechanism, also generally show severe memory deficits.
See also: , , and
Age-related conditions such as
and other forms of
(for which hippocampal disruption is one of the earliest signs) have a severe impact on many types of , but even normal aging is associated with a gradual decline in some types of memory, including
(or ). Because the hippocampus is thought to play a central role in memory, there has been considerable interest in the possibility that age-related declines could be caused by hippocampal deterioration. Some early studies reported substantial loss of neurons in the hippocampus of , but later studies using more precise techniques found only minimal differences. Similarly, some
studies have reported shrinkage of the hippocampus in elderly people, but other studies have failed to reproduce this finding. There is, however, a reliable relationship between the size of the hippocampus an so that where there is age-related shrinkage, memory performance will be impaired. There are also reports that memory tasks tend to produce less hippocampal activation in the elderly than in the young. Furthermore, a
published in 2011 found that
could increase the size of the hippocampus in adults aged 55 to 80 and also improve spatial memory.
The hippocampus contains high levels of , which make it more vulnerable to
than most other . There is evidence that humans having experienced severe, long-lasting traumatic stress show atrophy of the hippocampus more than of other parts of the brain. These effects show up in , and they may contribute to the hippocampal atrophy reported in
and . A recent study has also revealed atrophy as a result of depression, but this can be stopped with anti-depressants even if they are not effective in relieving other symptoms.
Chronic stress resulting in elevated levels of glucocorticoids, notably of cortisol, is seen to be a cause of neuronal atrophy in the hippocampus.This atrophy results in a smaller hippocampal volume which is also seen in . The higher levels of cortisol in Cushing’s syndrome is usually the result of medications taken for other conditions. Neuronal loss also occurs as a result of impaired neurogenesis. Another factor that contributes to a smaller hippocampal volume is that of dendritic retraction where dendrites are shortened in length and reduced in number, in response to increased glucocorticoids. This dendritic retraction is reversible. After treatment with medication to reduce cortisol in Cushing’s syndrome, the hippocampal volume is seen to be restored by as much as 10%. This change is seen to be due to the reforming of the dendrites. This dendritic restoration can also happen when stress is removed. There is, however, evidence derived mainly from studies using rats that stress occurring shortly after birth can affect hippocampal function in ways that persist throughout life.
Sex-specific responses to stress have also been demonstrated in the rat, to have an effect on the hippocampus. Chronic stress in the male rat showed dendritic retraction and cell loss in the CA3 region but this was not shown in the female. This was thought to be due to neuroprotective ovarian hormones.
Image 10: An EEG showing epilepsy right-hippocampal seizure onset
Image 9: An EEG showing epilepsy left-hippocampal seizure onset
The hippocampus is one of the few brain regions where new neurons are generated. This process of
is confined to the dentate gyrus. The production of new neurons can be positively affected by exercise or negatively affected by .
Seizures in
can affect the normal development of new neurons and can cause tissue damage.
is the most common type of such tissue damage. It is not yet clear, however, whether the epilepsy is usually caused by hippocampal abnormalities or whether the hippocampus is damaged by cumulative effects of seizures. However, in experimental settings where repetitive seizures are artificially induced in animals, hippocampal damage is a frequent result. This may be a consequence of the concentration of excitable
in the hippocampus. Hyperexcitability can lead to
and cell death. It may also have something to do with the hippocampus being a site where
continue to be created throughout life, and to abnormalities in this process.
The causes of
are not well understood, but numerous abnormalities of brain structure have been reported. The most thoroughly investigated alterations involve the cerebral cortex, but effects on the hippocampus have also been described. Many reports have found reductions in the size of the hippocampus in schizophrenic subjects. The left hippocampus seems to be affected more than the right. The changes noted have largely been accepted to be the result of abnormal development. It is unclear whether hippocampal alterations play any role in causing the psychotic symptoms that are the most important feature of schizophrenia. It has been suggested that on the basis of experimental work using animals, hippocampal dysfunction might produce an alteration of dopamine release in the , thereby indirectly affecting the integration of information in the . It has also been suggested that hippocampal dysfunction might account for the disturbances in long-term memory frequently observed.
MRI studies have found a smaller brain volume and larger ventricles in people with schizophrenia. Researchers do not know if the shrinkage is from the schizophrenia or from the medication. The hippocampus and thalamus have been shown to
and the volume of the globus pallidus is increased. Cortical patterns are altered, and a reduction in the volume and thickness of the cortex particularly in the frontal and temporal lobes has been noted. It has further been proposed that many of the changes seen are present at the start of the disorder which gives weight to the theory that there is abnormal neurodevelopment.
The hippocampus has been seen as central to the pathology of schizophrenia, both in the neural and physiological effects. It has been generally accepted that there is an abnormal synaptic connectivity underlying schizophrenia. Several lines of evidence implicate changes in the synaptic organization and connectivity, in and from the hippocampus Many studies have found dysfunction in the synaptic circuitry within the hippocampus and its activity on the prefrontal cortex. The glutamatergic pathways have been seen to be largely affected. The subfield CA1 is seen to be the least involved of the other subfields, and CA4 and the subiculum have been reported elsewhere as being the most implicated areas. The review concluded that the pathology could be due to genetics, faulty neurodevelopment or abnormal neural plasticity. It was further concluded that schizophrenia is not due to any known neurodegenerative disorder.
is a dramatic, sudden, temporary, near-total loss of short-term memory. Various causes have been hypothesized including ischemia, epilepsy, migraine and disturbance of cerebral venous blood flow, leading to
of structures such as the hippocampus that are involved in memory.
There has been no scientific proof of any cause. However,
studies taken from 12–24 hours following an episode has shown there to be small dot-like lesions in the hippocampus. These findings have suggested a possible implication of CA1 neurons made vulnerable by metabolic stress.
Image 11: Drawing by
of a hippocampus stained using the
The hippocampus has a generally similar appearance across the range of mammals, from
such as the
such as humans. The hippocampal-size-to-body-size ratio broadly increases, being about twice as large for primates as for the echidna. It does not, however, increase at anywhere close to the rate of the -to-body-size ratio. Therefore, the hippocampus takes up a much larger fraction of the cortical mantle in rodents than in primates. In adult humans the volume of the hippocampus on each side of the brain is about 3.0 to 3.5 cm3 as compared to 320 to 420 cm3 for the volume of the neocortex.
There is also a general relationship between the size of the hippocampus and spatial memory. When comparisons are made between similar species, those that have a greater capacity for spatial memory tend to have larger hippocampal volumes. This relationship also extend in species where males and females show strong differences in spatial memory ability they also tend to show corresponding differences in hippocampal volume.
Non-mammalian species do not have a brain structure that looks like the mammalian hippocampus, but they have one that is considered
to it. The hippocampus, as pointed out above, is in essence part of the allocortex. Only mammals have a fully developed cortex, but the structure it evolved from, called the , is present in all vertebrates, even the most primitive ones such as the
or . The pallium is usually divided into three zones: medial, lateral and dorsal. The medial pallium forms the precursor of the hippocampus. It does not resemble the hippocampus visually because the layers are not warped into an S shape or enfolded by the dentate gyrus, but the homology is indicated by strong chemical and functional affinities. There is now evidence that these hippocampal-like structures are involved in spatial cognition in birds, reptiles, and fish.
In birds, the correspondence is sufficiently well established that most anatomists refer to the medial pallial zone as the "avian hippocampus". Numerous species of birds have strong spatial skills, in particular those that cache food. There is evidence that food-caching birds have a larger hippocampus than other types of birds and that damage to the hippocampus causes impairments in spatial memory.
The story for fish is more complex. In
fish (which make up the great majority of existing species), the forebrain is distorted in comparison to other types of vertebrates: Most neuroanatomists believe that the teleost forebrain is in essence everted, like a sock turned inside-out, so that structures that lie in the interior, next to the ventricles, for most vertebrates, are found on the outside in teleost fish, and vice versa. One of the consequences of this is that the medial pallium ("hippocampal" zone) of a typical vertebrate is thought to correspond to the lateral pallium of a typical fish. Several types of fish (particularly goldfish) have been shown experimentally to have strong spatial memory abilities, even forming "cognitive maps" of the areas they inhabit. There is evidence that damage to the lateral pallium impairs spatial memory.
It is not yet known whether the medial pallium plays a similar role in even more primitive vertebrates, such as sharks and rays, or even lampreys and hagfish. Some types of insects, and molluscs such as the octopus, also have strong spatial learning and navigation abilities, but these appear to work differently from the mammalian spatial system, so there is as yet no good reason to think that they have a common nor is there sufficient similarity in brain structure to enable anything resembling a "hippocampus" to be identified in these species. Some have proposed, however, that the insect's
may have a function similar to that of the hippocampus.
Hippocampus animation
Pigeon hippocampus (HP)
Morphology in four
Main hippocampal regions in
Cell number distribution across species
Cell number to body weight ratios in rodent species
Neuronal loss shown in rat CA1 following
The 2017 version of this article has passed academic
and been published in the journal WikiJournal of Medicine
The published version can be read and cited
and the peer review .
Published version
Wright M, et al. (2017). . WikiJournal of Medicine. 4 (1): 3. :.
References
Martin, JH (2003). . Neuroanatomy: text and atlas (third ed.). McGraw-Hill Companies. p. 382.  .
Amaral, D; Lavenex, P (2007). . In Anderson, P; Morris, R; Amaral, D; Bliss, T; I'Keefe. The hippocampus book (first ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. p. 37.  .
Anderson, P; Morris, R; Amaral, D; Bliss, T; O'Keefe, J (2007). . In Anderson, P; Morris, R; Amaral, D; Bliss, T; I'Keefe. The hippocampus book (first ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. p. 3.  .
preparation by László Seress in 1980.
Owen, CM; Howard, A; Binder, DK (December 2009). "Hippocampus minor, calcar avis, and the Huxley-Owen debate.". Neurosurgery. 65 (6): ; discussion 1104–5. :.  .
Kazlev, M.Alan. . www.kheper.net.
. www.dartmouth.edu.
Andersen, P Morris, R Amaral, D Bliss, T O'Keefe, John (2 November 2006). . Oxford University Press.
Alberts, Daniel Albert (2012). Dorland's illustrated medical dictionary. (32nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Saunders/Elsevier. p. 860.  .
Purves, Dale (2011). Neuroscience (5th ed.). Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer. pp. 730–735.  .
Purves, Dale (2011). Neuroscience (5. ed.). Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer. p. 590.  .
Purves, Dale (2011). Neuroscience (5th ed.). Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer. p. 171.  .
. neuroscience.uth.tmc.edu.
Finger, S (2001). "Defining and controlling the circuits of emotion". Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function. Oxford/NewYork: Oxford University Press. p. 286.  .
"Extrinsic projections from area CA1 of the rat hippocampus: olfactory, cortical, subcortical, and bilateral hippocampal formation projections". Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990. :.
Schiller, D.; Eichenbaum, H.; Buffalo, E. A.; Davachi, L.; Foster, D. J.; Leutgeb, S.; Ranganath, C. (2015). "Memory and Space: Towards an Understanding of the Cognitive Map". Journal of Neuroscience. 35 (41): 1. :.  .  .
Eichenbaum H (2001). "The hippocampus and declarative memory: Cognitive mechanisms and neural codes". Behavioural Brain Research. 127 (1): 199–207. :.
Buzsáki, Gy? Moser, Edvard I (2013). . Nature Neuroscience. 16 (2): 130–138. :.  .   .  .
Ito, R; Lee, AC (15 October 2016). "The role of the hippocampus in approach-avoidance conflict decision-making: Evidence from rodent and human studies.". Behavioural Brain Research. 313: 345–57. :.  .
Di Gennaro G, Grammaldo LG, Quarato PP, Esposito V, Mascia A, Sparano A, Meldolesi GN, Picardi A (Jun 2006). "Severe amnesia following bilateral medial temporal lobe damage occurring on two distinct occasions". Neurological Sciences. 27 (2): 129–33. :.  .
V et al. (1999). "Primary CA1 and conditionally immortal MHP36 cell grafts restore conditional discrimination learning and recall in marmosets after excitotoxic lesions of the hippocampal CA1 field.". Brain. 122: . :.
O'Neil, EB; Newsome, RN; Li, IH; Thavabalasingam, S; Ito, R; Lee, AC (11 November 2015). "Examining the Role of the Human Hippocampus in Approach-Avoidance Decision Making Using a Novel Conflict Paradigm and Multivariate Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging.". The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 35 (45): 15039–49. :.  .
Komisaruk, B. R. (1970). "Synchrony between limbic system theta activity and rhythmical behavior in rats". Journal of comparative and physiological psychology. 70 (3): 482.
Girardeau, G Karim B Sidney I W Gy?rgy Buzsá Micha?l B Zugaro (September 2009). "Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory". Nature Neuroscience. 12 (10): . :.  .
Ego-Stengel, Valé Matthew A. Wilson (January 2010). . Hippocampus. 20 (1): 1–10. :.   .  .
Kovacs KA, O'Neill J, Schoenenberger P, Penttonen M, Ranguel Guerrero DK, Csicsvari J (19 Nov 2016). . PLOS ONE. 11: e0164675. :.   .  .
Karl A, Schaefer M, Malta LS, D?rfel D, Rohleder N, Werner A (2006). "A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 30 (7): 1004–31. :.  .
Wright IC, , Woodruff PW, David AS, Murray RM, Bullmore ET (January 2000). . The American Journal of Psychiatry. 157 (1): 16–25. :.  .
Kempton MJ, Salvador Z, Munafò MR, Geddes JR, Simmons A, Frangou S, Williams SC (Jul 2011). "Structural neuroimaging studies in major depressive disorder. Meta-analysis and comparison with bipolar disorder". Archives of General Psychiatry. 68 (7): 675–90. :.  . see also MRI database at
Starkman, MN; Giordani, B; Gebarski, SS; Berent, S; Schork, MA; Schteingart, DE (15 December 1999). "Decrease in cortisol reverses human hippocampal atrophy following treatment of Cushing's disease.". Biological Psychiatry. 46 (12): . :.  .
Disorders, Institute of Medicine (US) Forum on Neuroscience and Nervous System (1 January 2011). . National Academies Press (US).
Conrad CD (2008). . Reviews in the Neurosciences. 19 (6): 395–411. :.   .  .
Ortiz, JB; McLaughlin, KJ; Hamilton, GF; Baran, SE; Campbell, AN; Conrad, CD (29 August 2013). . Neuroscience. 246: 409–21. :.   .  .
Ho BC, Andreasen NC, Ziebell S, Pierson R, Magnotta V (February 2011). . Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 68: 128–37. :.   .  .
Fusar-P Smieskova, R; Kempton, MJ; Ho, BC; Andreasen, NC; Borgwardt, S. (September 2013). . Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 37: . :.   .  .
Haukvik, UK; Hartberg, CB; Agartz, I (23 April 2013). "Schizophrenia--what does structural MRI show?". Tidsskrift for den Norske laegeforening : tidsskrift for praktisk medicin, ny raekke. 133 (8): 850–3. :.  .
Harrison, PJ; Eastwood, SL (2001). "Neuropathological studies of synaptic connectivity in the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia.". Hippocampus. 11 (5): 508–19. :.  .
Szabo, K (2014). "Transient global amnesia.". Frontiers of neurology and neuroscience. 34: 143–9. :.  .
Lewis SL (Aug 1998). "Aetiology of transient global amnesia". Lancet. 352 (9125): 397–9. :.  .
Chung CP, Hsu HY, Chao AC, Chang FC, Sheng WY, Hu HH (Jun 2006). "Detection of intracranial venous reflux in patients of transient global amnesia". Neurology. 66 (12): 1873–77. :.  .
Aboitiz F, Morales D, Montiel J (Oct 2003). "The evolutionary origin of the mammalian isocortex: towards an integrated developmental and functional approach". The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 26 (5): 535–52. :.  .
Amaral D, Lavenex P (2006). "Ch 3. Hippocampal Neuroanatomy". In Andersen P, Morris R, Amaral D, Bliss T, O'Keefe J. The Hippocampus Book. Oxford University Press.  .
Anagnostaras SG, Gale GD, Fanselow MS (2002).
(PDF). Hippocampus. 12 (4): 561–565. :.  .
Best PJ, White AM (1999). "Placing hippocampal single-unit studies in a historical context". Hippocampus. 9 (4): 346–51. :.  .
Bliss TV, Lomo T (Jul 1973). . The Journal of Physiology. 232 (2): 331–56. :.   .  .
Boyer P, Phillips JL, Rousseau FL, Ilivitsky S (Apr 2007). "Hippocampal abnormalities and memory deficits: new evidence of a strong pathophysiological link in schizophrenia". Brain Research Reviews. 54 (1): 92–112. :.  .
Broglio C, Gómez A, Durán E, Oca?a FM, Jiménez-Moya F, Rodríguez F, Salas C (Sep 2005). "Hallmarks of a common forebrain vertebrate plan: specialized pallial areas for spatial, temporal and emotional memory in actinopterygian fish". Brain Research Bulletin. 66 (4–6): 397–99. :.  .
Burke SN, Barnes CA (Jan 2006). "Neural plasticity in the ageing brain". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 7 (1): 30–40. :.  .
Buzsáki G (Nov 1986). "Hippocampal sharp waves: their origin and significance". Brain Research. 398 (2): 242–52. :.  .
Buzsáki G (1989). . Neuroscience. 31 (3): 551–70. :.  .
Buzsáki G, Chen LS, Gage FH (1990). "Spatial organization of physiological activity in the hippocampal region: relevance to memory formation". Progress in Brain Research. Progress in Brain Research. 83: 257–68. :.  .  .
Buzsáki G (Jan 2002).
(PDF). Neuron. 33 (3): 325–40. :.  .
Buzsáki G (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford University Press.  .
Ramón y Cajal S (1894). "The Croonian Lecture: La Fine Structure des Centres Nerveux". Proceedings of the Royal Society. 55 (331–335): 444–68. :.
Campbell S, Macqueen G (Nov 2004). . Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience. 29 (6): 417–26.   .  .
Cantero JL, Atienza M, Stickgold R, Kahana MJ, Madsen JR, Kocsis B (Nov 2003). . The Journal of Neuroscience. 23 (34): 1.  .
Carey B (). . The New York Times.
Chiu YC, Algase D, Whall A, Liang J, Liu HC, Lin KN, Wang PN (2004). "Getting lost: directed attention and executive functions in early Alzheimer's disease patients". Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 17 (3): 174–80. :.  .
(Sep 2003). "Epilepsy". The New England Journal of Medicine. 349 (13): 1257–66. :.  .
Cho RY, Gilbert A, Lewis DA (2005). "Ch 22. The neurobiology of schizophrenia". In Charney DS, Nestler EJ. Neurobiology of Mental Illness. Oxford University Press US.  .
Cenquizca LA, Swanson LW (Nov 2007). . Brain Research Reviews. 56 (1): 1–26. :.   .  .
Clark RE, Broadbent NJ, Squire LR (2005). . Hippocampus. 15 (2): 260–72. :.   .  .
Colombo M, Broadbent N (Jun 2000). "Is the avian hippocampus a functional homologue of the mammalian hippocampus?". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 24 (4): 465–84. :.  .
Cooke SF, Bliss TV (Jul 2006). "Plasticity in the human central nervous system". Brain. 129 (Pt 7): 1659–73. :.  .
de Olmos J, Hardy H, Heimer L (Sep 1978). "The afferent connections of the main and the accessory olfactory bulb formations in the rat: an experimental HRP-study". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 181 (2): 213–244. :.  .
Diana RA, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C (Sep 2007). "Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (9): 379–86. :.  .
Duvernoy HM (2005). . The Human Hippocampus (3rd ed.). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. p. 1.  .
Eichenbaum H, Otto TA, Wible CG, Piper JM (1991). "Ch 7. Building a model of the hippocampus in olfaction and memory". In Davis JL, Eichenbaum H,. Olfaction. MIT Press.  .
Eichenbaum H, Cohen NJ (1993). Memory, Amnesia, and the Hippocampal System. MIT Press.
Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C (2007). . Annual Review of Neuroscience. 30: 123–52. :.   .  .
Ekstrom AD, Kahana MJ, Caplan JB, Fields TA, Isham EA, Newman EL, Fried I (Sep 2003).
(PDF). Nature. 425 (6954): 184–88. :. :.  .
Erickson KI, et al. (Feb 2011). . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (7): . :. :.   .  .
Fanselow MS, Dong HW (Jan 2010).
(PDF). Neuron. 65 (1): 7–19. :.   .  .
Finger, S (2001). Origins of Neuroscience: A History of Explorations Into Brain Function. Oxford University Press US.  .
Garcia-Segura LM (2009). Hormones and Brain Plasticity. Oxford University Press US.  .
Woon FL, Sood S, Hedges DW (Oct 2010). "Hippocampal volume deficits associated with exposure to psychological trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: a meta-analysis". Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 34 (7): . :.  .
Gorwood P, Corruble E, Falissard B, Goodwin GM (Jun 2008). "Toxic effects of depression on brain function: impairment of delayed recall and the cumulative length of depressive disorder in a large sample of depressed outpatients". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 165 (6): 731–9. :.  .
Goto Y, Grace AA (Nov 2008). . Trends in Neurosciences. 31 (11): 552–8. :.   .  .
Gray, JA; McNaughton N (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. Oxford University Press.
Gross CG (Oct 1993). "Hippocampus minor and man's place in nature: a case study in the social construction of neuroanatomy". . 3 (4): 403–416. :.  .
Hampel H, Bürger K, Teipel SJ, Bokde AL, Zetterberg H, Blennow K (Jan 2008). "Core candidate neurochemical and imaging biomarkers of Alzheimer's disease". Alzheimer's & Dementia. 4 (1): 38–48. :.  .
Harrison PJ (Jun 2004). "The hippocampus in schizophrenia: a review of the neuropathological evidence and its pathophysiological implications". Psychopharmacology. 174 (1): 151–62. :.  .
Hebb DO (1949). Organization of Behavior: a Neuropsychological Theory. New York: John Wiley.  .
Huerta PT, Lisman JE (Aug 1993). "Heightened synaptic plasticity of hippocampal CA1 neurons during a cholinergically induced rhythmic state". Nature. 364 (6439): 723–5. :. :.  .
Jackson JC, Johnson A, Redish AD (Nov 2006). "Hippocampal sharp waves and reactivation during awake states depend on repeated sequential experience". The Journal of Neuroscience. 26 (48): 12415–26. :.  .
Jacobs LF, Gaulin SJ, Sherry DF, Hoffman GE (Aug 1990). . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 87 (16): 6349–52. :. :.   .  .
Jacobs LF (2003). "The evolution of the cognitive map". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 62 (2): 128–39. :.  .
Jung MW, Wiener SI, McNaughton BL (Dec 1994).
(PDF). The Journal of Neuroscience. 14 (12): .  .
Kahana MJ, Seelig D, Madsen JR (Dec 2001). "Theta returns". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 11 (6): 739–44. :.  .
K?tter R, Stephan KE (1997). "Useless or helpful? The "limbic system" concept". Reviews in the Neurosciences. 8 (2): 139–45. :.  .
Jo?ls M (Apr 2008). "Functional actions of corticosteroids in the hippocampus". European Journal of Pharmacology. 583 (2–3): 312–321. :.  .
Kuruba R, Hattiangady B, Shetty AK (Jan 2009). . Epilepsy & Behavior. 14 Suppl 1: 65–73. :.   .  .
Lubenov EV, Siapas AG (May 2009). "Hippocampal theta oscillations are travelling waves". Nature. 459 (7246): 534–9. :.  .
Maguire EA, Burgess N, Donnett JG, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD, O'Keefe J (May 1998). "Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network". Science. 280 (5365): 921–24. :. :.  .
Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, Good CD, Ashburner J, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD (Apr 2000). . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 97 (8): . :. :.   .  .
Malenka RC, Bear MF (Sep 2004). "LTP and LTD: an embarrassment of riches". Neuron. 44 (1): 5–21. :.  .
Matsumura N, Nishijo H, Tamura R, Eifuku S, Endo S, Ono T (Mar 1999). . The Journal of Neuroscience. 19 (6): 2381–93.  .
McNaughton BL, Battaglia FP, Jensen O, Moser EI, Moser MB (Aug 2006). . Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 7 (8): 663–78. :.  .
Mizunami M, Weibrecht JM, Strausfeld NJ (Dec 1998). "Mushroom bodies of the cockroach: their participation in place memory". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 402 (4): 520–37. :.  .
Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O'Keefe J (Jun 1982). "Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions". Nature. 297 (5868): 681–83. :. :.  .
Moser MB, Moser EI (1998). "Functional differentiation in the hippocampus". Hippocampus. 8 (6): 608–19. :.  .
Moser EI, Kropff E, Moser MB (2008). "Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 31: 69–89. :.  .
Nadel L, O'Keefe J, Black A (Jun 1975). "Slam on the brakes: a critique of Altman, Brunner, and Bayer's response-inhibition model of hippocampal function". Behavioral Biology. 14 (2): 151–62. :.  .
Nakazawa K, McHugh TJ, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S (May 2004). "NMDA receptors, place cells and hippocampal spatial memory". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 5 (5): 361–72. :.  .
Nieuwenhuys, R (1982). "An Overview of the Organization of the Brain of Actinopterygian Fishes". Am. Zool. 22 (2): 287–310. :.
O'Kane G, Kensinger EA, Corkin S (2004). "Evidence for semantic learning in profound amnesia: an investigation with patient H.M". Hippocampus. 14 (4): 417–25. :.  .
O'Keefe J, Dostrovsky J (Nov 1971). "The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat". Brain Research. 34 (1): 171–75. :.  .
O'Keefe, J; Nadel L (1978). . Oxford University Press.
Portavella M, Vargas JP, Torres B, Salas C (2002). "The effects of telencephalic pallial lesions on spatial, temporal, and emotional learning in goldfish". Brain Research Bulletin. 57 (3–4): 397–99. :.  .
Pearce JM (Sep 2001). . Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 71 (3): 351. :.   .  .
Pothuizen HH, Zhang WN, Jongen-Rêlo AL, Feldon J, Yee BK (Feb 2004). "Dissociation of function between the dorsal and the ventral hippocampus in spatial learning abilities of the rat: a within-subject, within-task comparison of reference and working spatial memory". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 19 (3): 705–712. :.  .
Prull MW, Gabrieli JD, Bunge SA (2000). "Ch 2. Age-related changes in memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective". In Craik FI, Salthouse TA. The handbook of aging and cognition. Erlbaum.  .
Rodríguez F, López JC, Vargas JP, Broglio C, Gómez Y, Salas C (2002). "Spatial memory and hippocampal pallium through vertebrate evolution: insights from reptiles and teleost fish". Brain Research Bulletin. 57 (3–4): 499–503. :.  .
Rolls ET, Xiang JZ (2006). "Spatial view cells in the primate hippocampus and memory recall". Reviews in the Neurosciences. 17 (1–2): 175–200. :.  .
Rosenzweig ES, Barnes CA (Feb 2003). "Impact of aging on hippocampal function: plasticity, network dynamics, and cognition". Progress in Neurobiology. 69 (3): 143–79. :.  .
Scoville WB, Milner B (Feb 1957). . Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 20 (1): 11–21. :.   .  .
Shettleworth SJ (2003). "Memory and hippocampal specialization in food-storing birds: challenges for research on comparative cognition". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 62 (2): 108–16. :.  .
Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Wilson MA, Barnes CA (1996). "Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences". Hippocampus. 6 (2): 149–76. :.  .
Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Permenter M, Archibeque M, Vogt J, Amaral DG, Barnes CA (Aug 2007). "EEG sharp waves and sparse ensemble unit activity in the macaque hippocampus". Journal of Neurophysiology. 98 (2): 898–910. :.  .
Sloviter RS (Feb 2005). "The neurobiology of temporal lobe epilepsy: too much information, not enough knowledge". Comptes Rendus Biologies. 328 (2): 143–53. :.  .
Smith DM, Mizumori SJ (2006). "Hippocampal place cells, context, and episodic memory". Hippocampus. 16 (9): 716–29. :.  .
Solstad T, Boccara CN, Kropff E, Moser MB, Moser EI (Dec 2008). "Representation of geometric borders in the entorhinal cortex". Science. 322 (5909): 1865–68. :. :.  .
Squire LR (Apr 1992). "Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans". Psychological Review. 99 (2): 195–231. :.  .
Squire, LR; Schacter DL (2002). The Neuropsychology of Memory. Guilford Press.
Squire LR (Jan 2009). . Neuron. 61 (1): 6–9. :.   .  .
Sutherland GR, McNaughton B (Apr 2000). "Memory trace reactivation in hippocampal and neocortical neuronal ensembles". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 10 (2): 180–86. :.  .
Sutherland RJ, Kolb B, Whishaw IQ (Aug 1982). "Spatial mapping: definitive disruption by hippocampal or medial frontal cortical damage in the rat". Neuroscience Letters. 31 (3): 271–6. :.  .
Sutherland RJ, Weisend MP, Mumby D, Astur RS, Hanlon FM, Koerner A, Thomas MJ, Wu Y, Moses SN, Cole C, Hamilton DA, Hoesing JM (2001). "Retrograde amnesia after hippocampal damage: recent vs. remote memories in two tasks". Hippocampus. 11 (1): 27–42. :.  .
Suzuki M, Hagino H, Nohara S, Zhou SY, Kawasaki Y, Takahashi T, Matsui M, Seto H, Ono T, Kurachi M (Feb 2005). "Male-specific volume expansion of the human hippocampus during adolescence". Cerebral Cortex. 15 (2): 187–93. :.  .
Vanderwolf CH (Dec 2001). "The hippocampus as an olfacto-motor mechanism: were the classical anatomists right after all?". Behavioural Brain Research. 127 (1–2): 25–47. :.  .
Vargas JP, Bingman VP, Portavella M, López JC (Nov 2006). "Telencephalon and geometric space in goldfish". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (10): 2870–78. :.  .
VanElzakker M, Fevurly RD, Breindel T, Spencer RL (Dec 2008). . Learning & Memory. 15 (12): 899–908. :.   .  .
Wechsler RT, Morss, AM, Wustoff, CJ, & Caughey, AB (2004). . Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. p. 37.  .
West MJ (1990). "Stereological studies of the hippocampus: a comparison of the hippocampal subdivisions of diverse species including hedgehogs, laboratory rodents, wild mice and men". Progress in Brain Research. Progress in Brain Research. 83: 13–36. :.  .  .
Wilson MA, McNaughton BL (Jul 1994). "Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep". Science. 265 (5172): 676–79. :. :.  .
Winson J (Jul 1978). "Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in spatial memory deficit in the rat". Science. 201 (4351): 160–63. :. :.  .
Per A Richard M David A Tim B John O'Keefe, eds. (2007). The Hippocampus Book. .  .
Dori D James J. Knierim, eds. (2014). Space,Time and Memory in the Hippocampal Formation. .  .
Henri M. D F. Cattin (2005). The Human Hippocampus: Functional Anatomy, Vascularization, and Serial Sections with MRI. Springer.  .
Howard Eichenbaum (2002). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory. Oxford University Press US.  .
edited by Patricia E. Sharp. (2002). Patricia E. Sharp, ed. The Neural Basis of Navigation: Evidence from Single Cell Recording. Springer.  .
Philippe Taupin (2007). The Hippocampus: Neurotransmission and Plasticity in the Nervous System. Nova Publishers.  .
John H Byrne, ed. (2008). Learning and Memory: A comprehensive reference. Elsevier.  .
Wikimedia Commons has media related to .
via BrainNavigator
Buzsaki G (2010). "Hippocampus". Scholarpedia. 6 (1): 1468. :.
and associated structures
: Hidden categories:

我要回帖

更多关于 sleep number北京销售 的文章

 

随机推荐